Proverbial poetry: its settings and syntax



Yüklə 6,58 Mb.
səhifə37/51
tarix09.08.2018
ölçüsü6,58 Mb.
#62171
1   ...   33   34   35   36   37   38   39   40   ...   51

to observe other connections which may surface. If new

connections come to light, they, too, must be formalized

and systematically scrutinized in light of the text. Such

a methodology allows one to read creatively and

deictically as one hunts for known patterns and suspects

that new ones may appear.

Concerning repetitional items, several levels were

employed by the wise men. Van Parunak, recently

developing the concept of cohesion in terms of

transitional techniques, writes that the similarity which

binds a section together may be a result of phonological,

morphological, lexical, syntactical, logical or rhetorical
similarities.1

First, phonologically, proverbs may be linked via

a common alphabetic letter (Prov 11:9-12b; 20:7-9,

24-26).2 While the common letter is most easily

recognized when it is initial, it may also be found in an

anadiplotic sense at the end of one line and the beginning

of the next (cf. Prov 10:17-18). The repetition may link

bi-colon to bi-colon (Prov 10:25-26) or it may join a

single stich to its pair (Prov 11:10a, 10b). Sometimes

the repetition may be within the stich (Prov 11:15a, where

the high frequency of ר's bonds the stich together as a

phonetic unit). Sometimes it may be the similar phonetic

sound, rather than an equivalent alphabetic symbol, which

is the repeated and cohesive feature (cf. 10:18 and the

repetition of sibilants ס, שׂ, שׁ). Methodologically, it

may be asked how one knows when the repetition of a letter

____________________

1H. Van Dyke Parunak, "Transitional Techniques in

the Bible," JBL 102 (1983):528. Cf. M. A. K. Halliday and

Ruqaiya Hasan, Cohesion in English (London: Longman,

1976).


2Crenshaw, "Prolegomena," p. 14. Crenshaw has a

very helpful list of seven structuring principles which he

has observed. He writes, "Various means of linking several

proverbs occur: a common letter (Pr. 11:9-12b; 20:7-9;

24-26); the same introductory word (Pr. 15:13-14, 16-17);

the same idea (Pr. 16); the use of an acrostic (Pr.

31:10-31); paradoxical unity (Pr. 26:4-5); and numbers (Pr.

30:24-28). Thematic units characterize later proverbs (Pr.

1-9) and Sirach . . . ." Our study will merely develop the

potential of this statement in terms of Proverbs 10-15.


is significant or insignificant (note in the preceding

eight words the ten-fold repetition of the letter "i"; yet

one should not be tempted to treat this text as reflecting

a tacit tendency toward alliteration). Bostrom, in his

superb attempt to expose the cohesiveness of Proverbs

10-15, notes many letter repetitions which provide the

individual proverbs and the proverb clusters with their

cohesion.1 Margalit, as cited above, provides some

parameters which, although these may still seem somewhat

speculative, will at least provide some minimum

requirements.2 Features of alliteration (consonance and

assonance) and rhyme should be examined since they may

serve to bind together single proverbs as well as

proverbial clusters. While the phonetic repetition itself

is objective, whether it is significant or not will be a

subjective evaluation which may be stated only in terms of

____________________

1Bostrom, Paronomasi I Den Aldre Hebreiska

Mashcallitteraturen, pp. 118ff. Bostrom's work has

manifested great insight but in some cases he may have

overstated his point.

2Margalit, "Introduction to Ugaritic Prosody," pp.

310-13. "To be significant, a letter should occur: (a) at

least three times per seven verse-unit verse; and/or (b)

twice in a single word or once in each of two adjacent

words (especially at the beginning); and/or (c) as repeated

sequence of two or more adjacent letters, not necessarily

in the same order, and not necessarily in the scope of a

single word" (p. 311). This writer will use this as a

minimum guideline and feels that the positioning of letters

should be more accounted for (initial, medial, and final).


varying degrees of probability.1

The second repetitional feature is the repetition

of lexical units. While Brown has correctly noticed that

such repetitions may provide cues for determining larger

structures, they may also be a means of binding a stich,

bi-colon, proverbial pair or string together. As noted

above, classical rhetoric has provided some terminology

for describing such repetitions: (1) anaphora (units with

the same start; e.g., Prov 10:2, 3; 11:5, 6); (2) epiphora

(units with the same final words; e.g., 11:10a, 11a);

(3) ploke (the first starts the same as the second ends);

and (4) anadiplosis (the first ends the same as the second

begins).2 It has been observed that in both Egypt and

Mesopotamia the sages frequently used a catch-word

principle by which they bound proverbial pairs and strings

together (e.g., Prov 26:20, 21). Numerous writers have

noted this phenomenon in Proverbs (Murphy being the most

thorough and easily accessed).3 This feature is

particularly striking when the word is in the same

____________________



1E. D. Hirsch, Validity in Interpretation (New

Haven: Yale University Press, 1967), pp. 13-22. Hirsch

has a nice discussion on conscious and unconscious

authorial intent and the relationship of these to verbal

meaning.

2O'Connor, Hebrew Verse Structure, p. 144.

3Murphy, Wisdom Literature: Job, Proverbs,

Ruth,

Canticles, Ecclesiastes, Esther, pp. 68-73. Murphy's

analysis of cohesive features is the most complete in the

English language.
syntactic position or when it is a low frequency word

(e.g., Prov 10:14, 15 [מְחִתָּה (ruin)]; 10:32; 11:1 [רָצוֹן

(delight)]). If the analysis were to be complete, one

should monitor not only the fact that the repetition

exists, but also how it functions. Numerous variations

may be seen in the way the scribe creatively used

repetition to bind. Quite frequently he repeated an

item--thereby binding the proverb together--yet linked it

to its opposite (e.g., Prov 10:5 בֵּן מַשְׂכִיל (wise son); בֵּן

מֵבִישׁ (shameful son); 10:11 פִי צַדּיק [mouth of the

righteous]; פִי רְשָׁעִים [mouth of the wicked]).

Another feature to be included in the catch-word

or word repetition category is the repetition of larger

units (phrases, clauses, stichs and even whole proverbs).

Often the repetitions are with variation (e.g., 10:2a;

11:4a) or they may be exact repetitions (10:6b, 11b; and

10:8b, 10b).

One should not ignore variational techniques which

accompany the repetition. Often the repeated lexical root

will be found in a different syntactical position (note

בְּרָכָה [blessing] in Prov 10:6, 7). Variation may be

accomplished by morphological shifts in person, gender or

number (e.g., יְכַסֶּה / תְּכַסֶּה [conceal], Prov 10:11, 12).

The use of word pairs should be mentioned at this

point, as they provide a close parallel to exact

repetition. The sage often used word pairs to bind his


proverb together (e.g., שִׂנְאָה [hatred]/ אַהֲבָה [love], Prov

10:12). Frequently the paired word is in construct with a

word which turns the pair into an antithesis (e.g., כַף־

רְמִיָּה [lazy palm]/ יַד חָרוּצִים [diligent hand], Prov 10:4).

A third area of repetition is on a syntactic

level--whether in terms of deep or surface structure.

Proverbs 10:1b has been shown to echo syntactically 10:1a

via a nominalizing transformation which accounts for the

surface structure differences. Proverbs 10:5 can be shown

to be a perfect isomorphic syntactical match. So, too,

one may detect syntactic parallels between proverbs (e.g.,

Prov 10:31a, 32a; and 10:6, 7 with some variation).

Variations may include changes in person, gender, number

(Prov 10:2, 3, רֶשַׁע [wicked, singular] and רְשָׁעִים [wicked,

plural]). Most frequently in narrative there is the

continuity of pronominal markers which indicate unity (cf.

Prov 10:22).

A final area of repetition is topical--where one

proverb is thematically cohesive with its neighbor. While

it has been noted above that many writers recognize the

topical chaos of Proverbs 10-15, there are points of

topical coherence. Proverbs 10:2 and 3, for example, both

talk about wealth. Proverbs 10:18-21 comment on the

proper/improper use of speech.

Generally three types of proverbial clustering

have been observed. Though the proverbs are often atomic
and singular kernels, they are frequently found in paired

relationships. Proverbs 26:4, 5 is notorious because it

presents a paradoxical pair. Proverbs 10:2, 3 and 10:15,

16 (cf. also Prov 11:5, 6) are clear examples of

proverbial pairs about wealth. The second type shall be

designated as a proverbial string, which is a group of

three or more proverbs linked by any of the above

cohesional devices. A string may cohere on the basis of

topic (Prov 10:18-21) or by one of the above repetitional

features (Prov 11:9-11). Finally, several broken or

detached string elements have been noticed which may

provide a "hinging" effect between the string and its

context (Prov 11:9-11, 14; and Prov 10:23, 25-26).1

Thus, repetitional features may take the form of

sounds/letters, lexical units, phrases, clauses, or whole

proverbs. Particularly frequent are catch-words. In

addition to topical similarities, syntactical repetitions

and cohesions may also bind the text. To each of these

elements of equivalence (semantic, syntactic, phonetic)

there may be variations either from within the category

itself (repetition of a sibilant by the use of various

letters ס, שׂ, שׁ) or from another category (repetition of a

lexical root which is fitted to another syntactical or

morphological class).

____________________

1Van Parunak, "Transitional Techniques in the

Bible," pp. 540-46.


Sequential features may also provide unity for a

passage. The acrostic is a classic example of this on a

phonological level. The numerical proverbs are

sequentially bound by a numerical phenomenon (Prov

30:18-19). There may be a logical progression as a case

is argued or an event narrated, although such will not

occur explicitly in the corpus.

Hence, many elements of sequence and equivalence

will be monitored to determine if indeed this proverbial

collection was crafted according to principles or whether

it is merely a haphazard agglomeration of atomic proverbs

with no molecular inter-proverbial bonds. Still remaining

is to examine the text of Proverbs 10 itself, which will

provide the specimen for this experiment.


Cohesional Features in Proverbs 10
In order to facilitate a lucid discussion, there

will be a verse-by-verse monitoring of both intra- and

inter-proverbial cohesions. Concluding the discussion

will be the structural diagrams synthesizing these

cohesive factors. Because of the clarity of the diagrams,

it may be of benefit to refer to the diagrams as the

verses are discussed. One may wish to consult Bostrom

concerning letter/sound repetitions1 and Murphy for catch-

____________________

1Bostrom, Paronomasi I den Aldre Hebreiska

Mashcalliteraturen, pp. 118ff.
words and logical links.1 Since the tagmemic analysis has

carefully exposed the intra-proverbial syntax, these

features will not be mentioned at this point.

Proverbs 10:1 בֵּן חָכָם יְשַׂמַּח־אַב

A wise son brings joy to his father,
וּבֵן כְּסִיל תּוּגַת אִמּוֹ

but a foolish son grief to his mother.


Proverbs 10:1 is bound together syntactically and

via the familial terms (the repeated use of בֵּן [son]) and

the pairing of אַב (father) and אִמּוֹ (his mother). Each

stich seems to manifest an inclusio effect, by being

framed with familial terms (בֵּן, אַָב; and בֵּן, אִמּוֹ) thereby

foregrounding--by juxtaposition--the close nexus between



חָכָם and יְשַׂמַּח, and כְּסִיל; and תּוּגַת. It is possible that this

inclusio effect is further ameliorated by the repeated

consonants in 10:1a-- ב, ח, מ, מ, ח, ב. While this may

not be significant it does fit Margalit's standards

for alliteration. The repetitions of the letters

and the chiastic ordering have been previously noted by

____________________

1Murphy, Wisdom Literature: Job, Proverbs, Ruth,

Canticles, Ecclesiastes, Esther, pp. 68-73. The following

analysis reflects insights gained from the present writer's

extended exposure to linguistics, not from the work of

Bostrom (a copy of which was obtained only after the

analysis had been completed), or Murphy (whose work was

published after the following analysis was completed).

This writer does view their works as somewhat mutually

exclusive since Bostrom focuses on sound patterns and

Murphy on semantics. They are confirmatory to the general

thesis proposed here, i.e., that there is evidence of

collectional construction.

Bostrom.1 The repetition of the word בֵּן (son) in the

second stich results in the second stich's beginning with

a as well. While such sound/letter patterns may be of

no significance, they should be monitored since sometimes

they are clearly intentional. Intentionality most likely

was not involved in 10:1, however.

Proverbs 10:2 לֹא־יוֹעִילוּ אוֹצְרוֹת רֶשַׁע

Ill-gotten treasures are of no value,
וּצְדָקָה תַּצִיל מִמַּוֶת

but righteousness delivers from death.


Bostrom suggests that Proverbs 10:2 (cf. 11:4)

exhibits assonance.2 Note the four-fold repetition of the



וֹ ("o" sound) in the first stich. Also between the first

and second stichs is the יל sequence with a in the

immediate vicinity. The thrice-repeated fits the

alliteration standards, although it seems rather weak.

The semantical play on אוֹצְרוֹת (riches) being of no

יוֹעִילוּ (value) focuses on the two terms רֶשַׁע / צְדָקָה which

are drawn together both positionally and semantically for

contrast. Deliverance from death provides the benefits

that wealth, whether good or evil, could never attain.

Thus, the pragmatic value of צְדָקָה is unique. Again one

sees how well-crafted the sayings are.

____________________

1Bostrom, Paronomasi I den Aldre Hebreiska

Mashcalliteraturen, p. 120.

2Ibid., p. 120.

Proverbs 10:3 לֹא־יַרְעִיב יְהוָה נֶפֶש צַדִיק

The LORD does not let the righteous go hungry
וְהַוַת רְשָׁעִים יֶהְדֹף

But he thwarts the craving of the wicked.


Proverbs 10:3 obviously forms a pair with 10:2.

The introductory לֹאo followed by a Hiphil imperfect

unquestionably syntactically binds the two verses

together. The similarity does not stop there, however.

There is also a common thematic element, in that both

address the issue of the relationship of the

wicked/righteous to material possessions. This pair

provides an example of complex chiasm, as the initial

negations plus the imperfect verb would render the verbal

structure AB/AB, contrasting the wealth of the wicked,

whose wealth is valueless, with the righteous/

righteousness who receive material blessings from Yahweh

(לֹא־יוֹעִילוּ, תַּצִיל // לֹא־יַרְעִיב, יֶהְדֹף). In the person being

discussed, however, an AB/BA pattern (רֶשַׁע, צדָקָה;// צַדִּיקa,



רְשָׁעִים). Thus the repetition of righteous/righteousness

and wicked semantically binds these two sayings. They are

both concerned with a similar topic and similar character

qualities. Notice that the semantic elements of

equivalence (righteous/wicked) are varied morphologically

(רֶשַׁע, רְשָׁעִים, and צדָקָה;, צַדִּיקa). Bostrom notices the

repetition of the letter and particularly the sequence

יה in the divine name יְהוָה and in the verb of the second

stich, which YHWH does (יֶהְדֹף). Another linking feature is

the presence of the divine name in the first stich and the

pronominal reference back to it in the second. This

morphologically binds the proverbial bi-colon together

through one actor (יְהוָה), whose actions vary based on the

character of the individuals involved. A chiastic effect

is also contained in Proverbs 10:3 via the juxtaposing of

the characters (נֶפֶש צַדִיק / הַוַת רְשָׁעִים) with the imperfect

verbs framing the proverb (לֹא־יַרְעִיב, יֶהְדֹף). So there is

an AB/BA structure in the sequence: imperfect verb

describing God's actions/person involved//person

involved/imperfect verb describing God's actions. One

also wonders whether there is a play between נֶפֶש ("soul"

or "passion") and הַוַת (desire). Therefore, the inner

coherence, as well as, in this case, the bond with the

neighboring proverb, demonstrates the intricate

craftsmanship manifested in this saying and its pair

(10:2).

Proverbs 10:4 רָאשׁ עֹשֶׁה כַף־רְמִיָּה

Lazy hands make a man poor,
וְיַד חָרוּצִים תַעֲשִׁיר

but diligent hands bring wealth.


This verse continues the theme of material

possessions and suggests how wealth is properly accrued.

Bostrom well notes the alliteration with the "r" sounds,

as ר is repeated four times in the proverb.1 The proverb

____________________

1Ibid., p. 121.

begins and ends with r. It may be significant that both

verbs have an Wf sequence (עֹשֶׁה / תַּעֲשִׁיר). There is a

conspicuous chiastic structure with the inner elements

contrasting the character and the outer elements the

resultant economic status (poor/lax hand//diligent

hand/gets wealth). The middle terms are bound in that

and יַד are a standard word pair and are used here in a

synonymous manner. The contrast comes in the constructed

elements (רְמִיָּה / חָרוּצִים, cf. 10:1). Thus, the proverb itself

is a tightly-knit unit. Perhaps Bostrom is right when he

suggests that there is a word play in the sound-echoing

effect of חָרוּצִים with the word for gold (חָרוּץ).1

Proverbs 10:5 אֹגֶר בּקָיִץ בֵּן מַשְׂכִּיל

He who gathers crops in summer is a wise son,
נִרְדָּם בַּקָצִיל בֵּן מֵבִישׁ

but he who sleeps during harvest is a disgraceful son.


Proverbs 10:5 continues the theme of the acquiring

of wealth through diligence, thus indicating that 10:4 and

5 are also a proverbial pair. Again, as in 10:2, 3, there

is a bi-proverbial chiasm AB/BA (lax hands/diligent

hands//working wise son/otiose shameful son).

Syntactically, 10:5 is a total isomorphism and manifests a

strong syntactic cohesion within the proverb itself. The

word play between קַיִץ (summer) and קָצִיר (harvest) is an

obvious sonant-semantic play which further binds the

stichs together (cf. Prov 26:1; Amos 8:1-2). The

____________________

1Ibid.

five-fold repetition of is significant, especially when

it occurs four times in the word initial position. The

word repetitions of the preposition בְּ (in) and בֵּן (son)

engender the feeling of sameness. Bostrom makes a

contribution at this point by noticing that the order of

the sounds ר, בק, בן, and מ-שׁ in both stichs demonstrates

the genius of the sage who provides such a sonantally,

semantically, and syntactically perfect match.1 The Qal

active participle עֹשֶׂה (make) in 10:4a may assonantally tie

to the Qal active participle אֹגֵר (gathers) which begins

10:5a.


One may at this juncture reflectively suggest that

Proverbs 10:2-5 forms a quatrain centering on the theme of


Yüklə 6,58 Mb.

Dostları ilə paylaş:
1   ...   33   34   35   36   37   38   39   40   ...   51




Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©genderi.org 2024
rəhbərliyinə müraciət

    Ana səhifə