Contact Linguistics. Chap



Yüklə 1,16 Mb.
səhifə2/62
tarix28.03.2023
ölçüsü1,16 Mb.
#103369
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   ...   62
Winford2003.IntroductiontoContactLinguistics

pauno rani mandy ever dickdus. Ki did tute kin it?
‘goose I ever saw. Where did you buy it?’

Source: Leland, Charles G. 1874. The Gypsies and their language, p. 208. London: Trübner & Co.


EXERCISE. Discuss the ways in which each extract differs from Standard English, and list the features that characterize each. In what sense would you say these are varieties or dialects of English?


Examples such as these can be multiplied. Indeed, there are in principle no limits (except those imposed by universal grammar) to what speakers of different languages will adopt and adapt from one another, given the right opportunity. How can we explain such phenomena? What combinations of social and linguistic influences conspire to produce them? What kinds of situation promote one type of outcome rather than another? Questions like these are all part of the subject matter of Contact Linguistics. Its objective is to study the varied situations of contact between languages, the phenomena that result and the interaction of linguistic and external ecological factors in shaping these outcomes. The diverse kinds of mixture, change, adaptation and restructuring that result from interaction between (the users of) different languages have long been of interest to linguists. At the same time, scholars in the social sciences have devoted much attention to the social aspects of contact between different linguistic groups. For instance, they have investigated the nature of group relationships and group loyalty and how they are reflected in processes of accommodation in some circumstances, and by divergence and conflict in others. These two broad lines of research have converged significantly over the last few decades, resulting in a new cross-disciplinary approach to language contact that attempts to integrate the social and the linguistic in a unified framework. To understand how this approach evolved, it is useful to survey briefly the history of research on language contact.




2. History of research on language contact.

The study of the effects of language contact has been a focal point of interest to linguists ever since the earliest period of scientific study of language in the nineteenth century. In fact, interest in the topic among students of language dates back much earlier than this. For instance, Schuchardt (1884:30) (cited by Clyne 1987:452) mentions G. Lucio's (1666) discussion of the mixture of Croatian and Romance dialects in Dalmatia based on Dalmatian records of the fourteenth century. During the hey-day of Historical Linguistic scholarship in the nineteenth century, research on language contact became an integral part of the field and played a vital role in debate over the nature of language change. As Clyne (1987:453) reminds us, it was a topic to which such great linguists as Müller (1875), Paul (1886), Schmidt (1872) and Schuchardt (1884), among others, devoted a great deal of their attention. It continued to be a central topic well into the twentieth century, and was addressed by Sapir (1921), Bloomfield (1933) and other early pioneers of structuralism. In the hey-day of structuralism during the 1940's to the 1960's, it became rather less central, though not completely marginalized.


The major impetus for the concern with language contact among historical linguists arose from disagreement about the part played by contact-induced change in the history of languages. There was intense debate among nineteenth century scholars as to whether the conventional Stammbaum or "family tree" model of genetic relationships among languages was compromised in any way by the growing evidence that many languages contained a mixture of elements from different source languages. The field split into two camps, though many scholars occupied a middle ground between the two. On the one hand there were those who maintained that language mixture -especially mixture in grammar - was rare if not non-existent and that each language evolved from a single parent as a result of purely internal developments over time. For instance, Müller (1875) claimed that languages with mixed grammar did not exist, and this belief in the impenetrability of grammatical systems was echoed later by scholars like Meillet (1921:82) and more recently by Oksaar (1972:492) (cited by Thomason & Kaufman 1988:2). On the other hand there were many scholars who were equally convinced that language mixture was not only possible, but clearly evidenced by actual cases of contact. For instance, Whitney (1881), responding to Müller, argued that both lexical and grammatical transfer occurred in cases of contact. In his (1884) paper, Schuchardt, the first great creolist and pioneer in the study of contact languages, provided numerous examples of structural mixture and contact-induced change from a variety of situations, including Slavic/German, Slavic/Italian, and Balkan contact, as well as pidgin and creole situations.
The evidence of mixture provided by these and other scholars posed a serious challenge to orthodox Stammbaum theory with its insistence on a single-parent source for every language and its belief that practically all language change resulted from internal causes. From another angle, the work of scholars like Schmidt (1872) also provided evidence that changes could enter languages as the result of diffusion from external sources - a process which his "wave" model of change attempted to capture. The issue of how contact affects “genetic” affiliation is still a highly controversial one today. On the one hand, “traditional” historical linguists argue that a distinction should be made between “normal” and “abnormal” transmission (Thomason & Kaufmann 1988:11). The former would apply to languages whose components can for the most part be traced back to a single source language, even if they might have been subject to some external influence in the past. Such languages lend themselves to reconstruction via the traditional comparative historical model of single-parent genetic affiliation and gradual internal change. The label “abnormal transmission” would then apply to mixed languages whose various subsystems cannot all be traced back to a single parent language. They result from "broken transmission" and therefore have no genetic links to other languages in the standard sense of the term (1988:11). Such cases include pidgins, creoles and bilingual mixed languages, the three major types of contact language referred to earlier. However, many scholars have challenged this approach. They point, for instance, to the fact that all languages are mixed to some extent, and that the processes of change found in highly mixed languages such as creoles can be found in varying degrees in the cases of so-called “normal” transmission (Mufwene 1998; Thurston 1994; DeGraff to appear). From this standpoint, it is perhaps unfortunate that contact-induced change and its outcomes are still viewed by many as secondary, even marginal, to the central pursuits of Historical-Comparative Linguistics.
Despite (or perhaps because of) the disagreement in the field, there developed during the nineteenth to mid twentieth century a strong tradition of research in contact induced change, both within the ambit of Historical Linguistics, and in other disciplines. In addition to the theoretical issues referred to above, research within the former field, research focussed on specific geographic areas of contact; linguistic processes and types of contact-induced change; specific instances of mixture such as bilingual code-switching or processes of pidgin and creole formation; and the possible constraints on contact-induced change. Most of the current topics in the field were already the object of serious enquiry as early as the 19th century. For instance, the language situation in the Balkans has attracted the attention of scholars since Kopitar (1829 ) and Schuchardt (1884), and there is a considerable body of research on this linguistic area. Troubetzkoy (1928) [cited in Weinreich 1953:112, fn 4] provided the first definition of a Sprachbund, “union of languages” or “linguistic area”) and since then there have been numerous studies of linguistic areas around the world. Other topics such as lexical borrowing and the role of substratum influence (discussed later) in language change were investigated. And of course much attention was paid to pidgins and creoles, as classic examples of "new" mixed languages. Schuchardt's pioneering work in this field was complemented by that of Hesseling (1899, 1905), Broch (1927) and others. Early in the 20th century, the phenomenon of code switching was studied by Braun (1937), who observed switches between Russian and German in the speech of a bilingual.
This line of more linguistically oriented research was complemented by other approaches concerned more with the social context of language contact. For instance, some scholars devoted their attention to the problems of long-established ethnic minorities faced with the strong influence of a majority national language. Systematic study of language maintenance began with Kloss (1927, 1929). Other scholars became interested in the fate of immigrant languages in North America and elsewhere (Herzog 1941, Reed 1948, Pap 1949, etc.). Studies like these established the foundation for the discipline known as the Sociology of Language, focussing on Language Maintenance and Shift (See Fishman 1964, Fishman et al. 1966). It provided important insights into the social and psychological factors that determine the outcomes of language contact. Closely associated with this tradition is the growing body of research on the social psychology of language choice as exemplified, for instance, by the approach known as Speech Accommodation Theory, developed by Howard Giles and his associates (Street & Giles 1982). Within the Historical Linguistics tradition too, many scholars stressed the importance of social factors in language contact. They included Whitney (1881) and Schuchardt (1884), who was in many ways far ahead of his time. Much of Schuchardt's discussion of the linguistic aspects of language contact is accompanied by details of the social context, the groups in contact, and other relevant socio-cultural data.
New vigor was injected into the field by the important work of Weinreich (1953) and Haugen (1950a&b; 1953). Working within the structural paradigm, they both emphasized the importance of studying language contact from both a linguistic and socio-cultural perspective. Clyne (1987:453) suggests that their work can be considered the beginning of American Sociolinguistics. If so, it is also true that their work established the groundwork for the re-emergence of language contact as a topic of central importance and as a subdiscipline of Linguistics in its own right.
All of these various lines of approach, some primarily linguistic, others primarily sociological or anthropological, contributed to the emergence of the new field of Contact Linguistics. According to Nelde (1997:287), the term was introduced at the First World Congress on Language Contact and Conflict, held in Brussels in June 1979. As noted earlier, the major turning point in the discipline was the work of Haugen and Weinreich, particularly the latter. As Clyne (1987:456) notes, despite all the previous research, "there was, before Weinreich (1953), no systematized theory of language contact." Both he and Haugen attempted to integrate linguistic analysis with social and psychological explanations to account for language contact and its consequences. Their major contribution to this enterprise was undoubtedly their formulation of a comprehensive framework for the study of language contact in its social setting. Perhaps the strongest recent impetus to research in this area came from Thomason & Kaufman's (1988) book-length study of a wide variety of contact phenomena, and their attempt to lay the foundations for both a typology of contact outcomes and an empirical/theoretical framework for analyzing such outcomes. Their work constitutes a major contribution to Historical Linguistic scholarship, in attempting to resolve the old controversy over the role of external linguistic influence as distinct from internal motivations and mechanisms in language development. Like earlier researchers, they emphasized the need for an interdisciplinary approach and refined several aspects of the terminology and descriptive framework employed in previous studies. The emerging field of Contact Linguistics owes its existence primarily to the work of all these pioneers.


3. The field of Contact Linguistics.

Despite Appel & Muysken's (1987:7) assertion that "Bilingualism or language contact in itself is not a scientific discipline", the study of language contact is in fact a fairly well-defined field of study, with its own subject-matter and objectives. It employs an eclectic methodology that draws on various approaches, including the comparative-historical method, and various areas of Sociolinguistics. It is this very interdisciplinary approach that defines it and gives it its strength. One of the clearest statements of the goals of this subdiscipline is the following, from Weinreich (1953:86).


To predict typical forms of interference from the sociolinguistic description of a bilingual community and a structural description of its languages is the ultimate goal of interference studies.


Though Weinreich focuses specifically on the phenomenon of bilingualism, his statement can, mutatis mutandis, apply equally well to the study of all contact situations. Moreover, the field of Contact Linguistics is not limited to just the study of “interference,” but covers all the linguistic consequences of contact, including phenomena such as simplification and various other kinds of restructuring that characterize the outcomes of contact. Weinreich's goal of "prediction" is perhaps ambitious, but he himself is well aware of the complexity of the problem. In particular, he emphasizes that the components of an explanatory framework must include "purely structural considerations ... psychological reasons .... and socio-cultural factors" (1953:44). The need to explore the latter two types of factor arises from the fact that, first, contact situations which appear quite similar in terms of the linguistic inputs present, can and do result in quite different linguistic outcomes. Moreover, for any given contact situation, predictions of contact-induced changes based solely on structural factors fail miserably. This point will be discussed in later chapters, when we consider the various linguistic constraints on such changes. Weinreich's outline of the main concerns of “interference” studies is worth quoting in full. He notes:


In linguistic interference, the problem of major interest is the interplay of structural and non-structural factors that promote or impede such interference. The structural factors are those which stem from the organization of linguistic forms into a definite system, different for every language and to a considerable degree independent of non-linguistic experience and behavior. The non-structural factors are derived from the contact of the system with the outer world, from given individuals' familiarity with the system, and from the symbolic value which the system as a whole is capable of acquiring and the emotions it can evoke. (1953:5).


It follows, first, that we need to distinguish among the various social contexts of language contact if we are to understand the nature and direction of contact-induced change. Second, it is necessary to examine, where possible, the actual speech behavior of persons in each contact situation in order to uncover the factors that motivate them to change their language in one way or another.


Scholars have long been aware that differences in the social setting lead to differences in the outcomes of contact. For instance, Wackernagel (1904) distinguished three kinds of contact situation - when a conquered group adopts the language of its conquerors, when the reverse occurs, and when there is mutual influence leading to a "mixed language." Every outcome of language contact has associated with it a particular kind of social setting and circumstances that shape its unique character. The goal of Contact Linguistics is to uncover the various factors, both linguistic and socio-cultural, that contribute to the linguistic consequences of contact between speakers of different language varieties. Toward that end, we need a framework of analysis that includes a variety of components. In the rest of this chapter, we provide a broad overview of types of contact situation, their outcomes, and the social settings in which they emerge. We will consider each of these situations in more detail in subsequent chapters. There too we will explore the mechanisms and types of change involved as well as the factors, both linguistic and non-linguistic, which influence the patterns of cross-linguistic influence.


4. Types of contact situation.

We can in general distinguish three broad kinds of contact situation: those involving language maintenance, those involving language shift, and those that lead to the creation of new contact languages. Most cases of language contact can be assigned clearly to one or another of these categories. However, as we will see, there are many situations that cannot be classified so readily. Some are characterized by interplay between maintenance and shift, like the “fuzzy” cases found in Sprachbünde or linguistic areas such as the Balkans, discussed in Chapter 3. Others involve types of interaction and mutual accommodation which make it difficult to place them in a single category, for instance the kinds of extreme structural convergence found in North West New Britain, where languages of the Austronesian and non-Austronesian families have become structurally isomorphic (see Chapter 3). Similar difficulties arise in the case of the so-called “new” contact languages, pidgins (Chapter 8) , creoles (Chapter 9) and bilingual mixed languages (Chapter 6). These are cases neither of maintenance nor shift in the strict sense, though they share characteristics in common with the latter situations. Each of them presents its own problems of definition and classification.




4.1. Situations of language maintenance.


4.1.1: Borrowing situations.

Yüklə 1,16 Mb.

Dostları ilə paylaş:
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   ...   62




Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©genderi.org 2024
rəhbərliyinə müraciət

    Ana səhifə