Contact Linguistics. Chap



Yüklə 1,16 Mb.
səhifə4/62
tarix28.03.2023
ölçüsü1,16 Mb.
#103369
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   ...   62
Winford2003.IntroductiontoContactLinguistics

Exercise: Rayfield predicts that in situations of second language learning, lexical borrowing from the L2 will be much more frequent than structural borrowing in the L1 of the learners, while structural changes due to L1 influence will be more frequent in the learner’s version of the L2. Investigate the use of English or any other language as a second language by international students at your university. Does Rayfield’s prediction hold true as far as their usage is concerned?


4.3. Language creation: New contact languages.

In addition to maintenance and shift situations, there are other kinds of contact setting which have yielded rather special outcomes, the contact languages referred to as pidgins, creoles and bilingual mixed languages. These outcomes involve such extreme restructuring and/or such pervasive mixture of elements from more than one language that they cannot be considered cases of either maintenance or shift in the strict senses of those terms. It is also difficult at times to decide which outcomes of contact should be included in each of the above categories of contact language. The labels "pidgin" and "creole", for instance, have each been applied to a very heterogeneous group of languages, which differ both in the circumstances of their creation and in their structural characteristics. For this reason, it is necessary to refer to "prototypical" examples of each category, and attempt as far as possible to relate other potential members of the class to the prototype (Thomason 1997).




4.3.1. Bilingual mixed languages.
Bilingual mixed or intertwined languages arose in settings involving long-term contact between two ethnic groups leading to bilingualism and increasing mixture of the languages. In these cases, that mixture became conventionalized as a community norm, resulting in the creation of hybrid languages whose components could clearly be traced to one or the other source language. We saw one example of a bilingual mixed language, Anglo-Romani, earlier in this chapter. Another example is the Media Lengua of Ecuador, a language which incorporates Spanish lexicon into a virtually unchanged Quechua grammatical framework. The latter preserves intact not just the syntactic rules of Quechua, but also its highly complex morphology. Here is a brief example, in which a Media Lengua speaker explains how the language is made up. Items derived from Spanish are in italics.


Media Lengua-ga así Ingichu-munda Castallanu-da abla-na kiri-xu-sha,
Media Lengua-TOP thus Quechua-from Spanish-ACC talk-NOM want-PROG-SUB


no abla-naku-ndu-mi asi, chaupi-ga Castellanu laya, i chaupi-ga
not talk-pl-SUB-AFF thus, half-TOP Spanish like, and half-TOP

Ingichi laya abla-ri-na ga-n.


Quechua like talk-REFL-NOM-be-3.

“Media Lengua is thus if you want to talk Spanish from Quechua, but you can’t, then you talk half like Spanish, and half like Quechua.”


Source: Muysken, Pieter. 1997:377).


Other somewhat similar examples are Michif, a language in which Cree VP structure is wedded to French NP structure, and Mednyj Aleut, in which Russian finite verb morphology and other structural features have been fused with Aleut grammatical systems. In general, it is fair to say that these vernaculars fuse the grammar of one source with the lexicon (at least the phonological representations of the lexical items) of another. However, this picture is simplistic, since it ignores many respects in which a bilingual mixed language may differ from either of its source languages. Moreover, no single formula can be applied to describe or predict the mixture, even though there are many similarities in design among them. These and other aspects of the genesis and structure of bilingual mixed languages will be discussed further in Chapter 6.


4.3.2. Pidgins.

Trading contacts between groups speaking different languages have often led to various types of linguistic compromise to facilitate communication. Such compromises often result in pidgins, highly reduced languages with minimal vocabulary and grammar whose functions are restricted primarily to barter and exchange. An example of the pidgin English used for trading between English speakers and Pacific islanders in the 19th century was provided earlier in this chapter. Pidgins are a rather mixed bag of languages. Some involve more lexical mixture than others. For instance, Russenorsk, used in trade between Russians and Norwegians up to the nineteenth century, employed vocabulary from both groups' languages. Other pidgins, like Eskimo Trade Pidgin and Chinese Pidgin English, derive their vocabulary primarily from one source, Eskimo in the former, English in the latter. The primary source language in these cases tends to be the language of the group that has control of the trade or its location. Pidgins have also arisen in contexts other than trade, for instance in cases of military occupation (Pidgin English in Japan during the post-war period) or in domestic settings for communication between employers and servants of different language backgrounds (Indian Butler English) or on plantations (Hawaiian Pidgin English).


The cases mentioned so far are all examples of prototypical pidgins. The label is necessary because there is in fact a great deal of controversy over the scope of reference of the term "pidgin." The reason is that the degree of reduction in structure as well as range of functions may differ significantly from one case to another. Prototypical pidgins are severely restricted in terms of their social functions, and clearly reduced in form and structure, containing a minimal lexicon and a rudimentary grammar. Bickerton (1981) describes them as lacking inflectional morphology, tense/mood/aspect sytems, movement rules, embedding strategies and other structural characteristics associated with fully developed natural languages. The socio-historical and structural criteria by which such pidgins are defined will be outlined further in Chapter 8.
By contrast, other languages to which the term "pidgin" has been applied, for example, Tok Pisin, Nigerian Pidgin, etc, are far more elaborate in terms of social function and structure, and hardly meet the criteria for inclusion in this class. These more elaborate contact languages may be placed in two broad categories: extended pidgins, and simplified languages, though once more, the boundaries between these two are not always clear.
So-called extended pidgins apparently began as highly reduced (prototypical) pidgins which then underwent varying degrees of elaboration in both vocabulary and grammar when their range of functions extended beyond the confines of their original contexts of use. In such cases, there is usually incorporation of features from both the lexifier (superstrate) language and the native (substrate) languages of indigenous groups. Contact vernaculars like these can achieve such a degree of elaboration in this way that they become indistinguishable from other fully developed natural languages. Examples include Tok Pisin and Bislama, official languages of Papua-New Guinea and Vanuatu respectively, both descended from an earlier plantation pidgin, in turn rooted in early Pacific Trade Pidgin. Other examples include varietes of West African Pidgin English such as Nigerian Pidgin English, that are used as lingue franche in various parts of West Africa. These contact languages share much more in common, both functionally and structurally, with creoles than with prototypical pidgins.
There are other contact vernaculars to which the label "pidgin" has been applied which do not appear to involve the same degree of structural reduction characteristic of prototypical pidgins. For instance, languages like Trade Motu or Pidgin Yimas appear to be somewhat simplified forms of Motu and Yimas respectively, only partially reduced so as to facilitate their use by non-native speakers in trading and other contacts with native speakers. Their degree of reduction is not nearly as extensive as that found in, say, Russenorsk. Hence they should arguably be referred to as simplified languages, rather than pidgins. All of these cases and others like them will be discussed more fully in Chapter 8.


4.3.3. Creoles
European colonial expansion during the fifteenth to nineteenth centuries led in many cases to the creation of new communities peopled primarily by groups transplanted from distant regions of the world. In the plantations of the New World, where huge numbers of slaves were transplanted from West Africa, contact between the latter and European settlers led to the emergence of creole languages, so called because they were used by the creole or locally born descendants of slaves (as well as Europeans and other freemen) in the colonies.
A typical example is Sranan Tongo, a brief sample of which was provided earlier in this chapter. Other well-known Caribbean creoles include Jamaican and Guyanese creole (English lexicon); Haitian creole (French lexicon); Papiamentu, a creole used in the former Dutch islands of Aruba, Bonaire and Curacao (Spanish/Portuguese lexicon) and Berbice Dutch, once spoken in the interior of modern Guyana (Dutch lexicon).
Similar languages emerged in the Indian Ocean and other areas where European colonies were established. For instance, there is Isle de France creole, a French lexicon creole with varieties spoken in Mauritius and the Seychelles. In South East Asia, we find creoles such as Daman Creole Portuguese, spoken in India and Papia Kristang, spoken in Malaysia and Singapore. There are also several other creole languages spoken in West Africa, including Krio (English Lexicon) spoken in Sierra Leone and Guiné Kriyol (Portuguese lexicon) spoken in Guiné-Bissau. Some of the earliest creoles known arose on plantation settings on islands off the West African coast. Well-known examples include Cape Verde Crioulo and other Portuguese lexicon creoles spoken on São Tomé, Principe and other islands in the Gulf of Guinea.
The formation of these languages involved varying degrees of input from the superstrate languages of the colonizers and the native languages of the subjected peoples. Creoles, like other contact vernaculars, differ significantly in the nature and extent of the respective inputs. Just about every aspect of these languages, their origins and sources, their typological characteristics, their classification, etc., remains a matter of controversy. These issues will be discussed more fully in Chapter 9.
As with "pidgins", there are substantial differences among so-called "creoles" both in terms of their processes of formation and their structural make-up. Essentially, such differences have to do with the nature and extent of the substratum contribution to the creole's formation. On the one hand, there are radical creoles like Sranan and its Surinamese relatives Saramaccan, and varieties of the Eastern Maroon Creole, a substantial part of whose grammar can be traced to West African (especially Gbe) sources. For this reason, it is difficult to accept Thomason & Kaufman's characterization of them as cases of shift "whose structure can be accounted for under a hypothesis of extreme unsuccessful acquisition of a TL" (1988:48). One might just as well argue that they are akin to cases of maintenance, though, as usual, the truth lies somewhere between these two extremes.
By sharp contrast, the so-called intermediate creoles of the Caribbean such as Bajan, urban Guyanese or Trinidadian creole, are arguably cases of shift and far more akin to products of "unsuccessful" acquisition of a TL such as Hiberno English, Singapore English, Taiwanese Mandarin, etc., than they are to radical creoles. Once more, between these poles lie many other points on a continuum that includes contact vernaculars in the Caribbean, Pacific, Indian Ocean and elsewhere, to which the label "creole" has traditionally been applied.


5. Overview of contact situations and their outcomes..

At this point, it may be useful to provide a brief taxonomy of contact situations and the types of cross-linguistic influence they involve. Table 2, based partly on Thomason & Kaufman (1988:50), illustrates the major outcomes of language contact. The table distinguishes three general categories of outcome, those pertaining to language maintenance situations (here subdivided into borrowing and convergence situations), those relating to language shift and those involving the creation of new contact vernaculars, viz., pidgins, creoles and bilingual mixed languages.




Table 2: Major outcomes of Language Contact.


(A) Language maintenance.


I Borrowing situations:


Degree of contact. Linguistic Results Examples

Casual Lexical borrowing only. Modern English borrowings from


French. e.g. ballet.

Moderate Lexical and slight structural Latin influence on Early modern


borrowing. English. Sanskrit influence on
Dravidian languages.

Intense Moderate structural borrowing. German influence on Romansh.




II Situations of convergence.



Yüklə 1,16 Mb.

Dostları ilə paylaş:
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   ...   62




Verilənlər bazası müəlliflik hüququ ilə müdafiə olunur ©genderi.org 2024
rəhbərliyinə müraciət

    Ana səhifə